Skip to main content
Log in

The Weak Coherence Account: Detail-focused Cognitive Style in Autism Spectrum Disorders

  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

“Weak central coherence” refers to the detail-focused processing style proposed to characterise autism spectrum disorders (ASD). The original suggestion of a core deficit in central processing resulting in failure to extract global form/meaning, has been challenged in three ways. First, it may represent an outcome of superiority in local processing. Second, it may be a processing bias, rather than deficit. Third, weak coherence may occur alongside, rather than explain, deficits in social cognition. A review of over 50 empirical studies of coherence suggests robust findings of local bias in ASD, with mixed findings regarding weak global processing. Local bias appears not to be a mere side-effect of executive dysfunction, and may be independent of theory of mind deficits. Possible computational and neural models are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ameli R., Courchesne E., Lincoln A., Kaufman A. S., Grillon C., (1988). Visual memory processes in high-functioning individuals with autism Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 18:601–615

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition (DSM-IV) Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association

    Google Scholar 

  • Aurnhammer-Frith U., (1969). Emphasis and meaning in recall in normal and autistic children Language & Speech 12:29–38

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Barnea-Goraly N., Kwon H., Menon V., Eliez S., Lotspeich L., Reiss A. L., (2004). White matter structure in autism: Preliminary evidence from diffusion tensor imaging Biological Psychiatry 55:323–326

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen S., (2002). The extreme male brain theory of autism Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6:248–254

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen S., (2003). The essential difference: The truth about the male and female brain New York, NY Basic Books

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen S., Leslie A. M., Frith U., (1985). Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition 21:37–46

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright, S., Stott, C., Bolton, P., Goodyer, I., (1997). Is there a link between engineering and autism? Autism 1:101–109

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen S., Richler J., Bisarya D., Gurunathan N., Wheelwright S., (2003). The Systemising Quotient (SQ): An investigation of adults with Asperger Syndrome or High Functioning Autism and normal sex differences Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B, Special issue on “Autism: Mind and Brain” 358:361–374

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S., Griffin R., Lawson J., Hill J., (2002). The exact mind: Empathising and systemising in autism spectrum conditions In: U. Goswami, (Ed.). Handbook of cognitive development Malden, MA, USA Blackwell Publishers (pp. 491–508)

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartlett F. C., (1932). Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology Cambridge, England Cambridge University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Bellugi U., Lichtenberger L., Jones W., Lai Z., St. George M., (2000). The neurocognitive profile of Williams syndrome: A complex pattern of strengths and weaknesses Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12:7–29

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Belmonte M. K., Allen G., Beckel-Mitchener A., Boulanger L. M., Carper R. A., Webb S. J., (2004). Autism and abnormal development of brain connectivity Journal of Neuroscience 24:9228–9231

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Belmonte M. K., Yurgelun-Todd D. A., (2003). Functional anatomy of impaired selective attention and compensatory processing in autism Cognitive Brain Research 17:651–664

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Benowitz L. I., Moya K. L., Levine D. N., (1990). Impaired verbal reasoning and constructional apraxia in subjects with right hemisphere damage Neuropsychologia 23:231–241

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger H. J., Aerts F. H., van Spaendonck K. P., Cools A. R., Teunisse J. P., (2003). Central coherence and cognitive shifting in relation to social improvement in high-functioning young adults with autism Journal of Clinical & Experimental Neuropsychology 25:502–511

    Google Scholar 

  • Berger H. J., van Spaendonck K. P., Horstink M. W., Buytenhuijs E. L., Lammers P. W., Cools A. R., (1993). Cognitive shifting as a predictor of progress in social understanding in high-functioning adolescents with autism: A prospective study Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders 23:341–359

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bertone A., Mottron L., Jelenic P., Faubert J., (2003). Motion perception in autism: A “complex” issue Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 15:218–225

    Google Scholar 

  • Beversdorf, D. Q., Anderson, J. M., Manning, S. E., Anderson, S. L., Nordgren, R. E., Felopulos, G. J., Nadeau, S. E., Heilman, K. M., Bauman, M. L. (1998). The effect of semantic and emotional context on written recall for verbal language in high functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder Journal of Neurology,Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry 65:685–692

    Article  PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Beversdorf, D. Q., Smith, B. W., Crucian, G. P., Anderson, J. M., Keillor, J. M., Barrett, A. M., Hughes, J. D., Felopulos, G. J., Bauman, M. L., Nadeau, S. E., & Heilman, K. M. (2000). Increased discrimination of “false memories” in autism spectrum disorder Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 97:8734–8737

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Bonnel A., Mottron L., Peretz I., Trudel M., Gallun E., Bonnel A. M., (2003). Enhanced pitch sensitivity in individuals with autism: A signal detection analysis Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 15:226–235

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Booth R., Charlton R., Hughes C., Happé F., (2003). Disentangling weak coherence and executive dysfunction: Planning drawing in Autism and ADHD Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society 358(1430):387–392

    Google Scholar 

  • Brian J. A., Bryson S. E., (1996). Disembedding performance and recognition memory in autism/PDD Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 37:865–872

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Briskman J., Happé F., Frith U., (2001). Exploring the cognitive phenotype of autism: Weak ‘central coherence’ in parents and siblings of children with autism. II. Real-life skills and preferences Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 42:309–316

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Brock J., Brown C. C., Boucher J., Rippon G., (2002). The temporal binding deficit hypothesis of autism Development & Psychopathology 14:209–224

    Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan M. J., Scott F. J., Fox S., Pye J., (2004). Gestalt processing in autism: Failure to process perceptual relationships and the implications for contextual understanding Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 45:459–469

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Burack J. A., (1994). Selective attention deficits in persons with autism: Preliminary evidence of an inefficient attentional lens Journal of Abnormal Psychology 103:535–543

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Caron M. J., Mottron L., Rainville C., Chouinard S., (2004). Do high functioning persons with autism present superior spatial abilities? Neuropsychologia 42:467–481

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Castelli F., Frith C., Happé F., Frith U., (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes Brain 125:1839–1849

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chen Y., Nakayama K., Levy D., Matthysse S., Holzman P., (2003). Processing of global, but not local, motion direction is deficient in schizophrenia Schizophrenia Research 61:215–227

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chugani D. C., Muzik O., Behen M., Rothermel R., Janisse J. J., Lee J., Chugani H. T., (1999). Developmental changes in brain serotonin synthesis capacity in autistic and nonautistic children Annals of Neurology 45:287–295

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Chung M. K., Dalton K. M., Alexander A. L., Davidson R. J., (2004). Less white matter concentration in autism: 2D voxel-based morphometry Neuroimage 23:242–251

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chynn E. W., Garrod A., Demick J., DeVos E., (1991). Correlations among field dependence-independence, sex, sex-role stereotype, and age of preschoolers Perceptual and Motor Skills 73:747–756

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen I. L., (1994). An artificial neural network analogue of learning in autism Biological Psychiatry 36:5–20

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne E., (2004). Brain development in autism: Early overgrowth followed by premature arrest of growth Mental Retardation & Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews 10:106–111

    Google Scholar 

  • DeGelder B., Vroomen J., Van der Heide L., (1991). Face recognition and lip-reading in autism European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 3:69–86

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennis M., Lazenby A. L., Lockyer L., (2001). Inferential language in high-functioning children with autism Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 31:47–54

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Derryberry D., Tucker D. M., (1994). Motivating the focus of attention In: P. M. Neidenthal, S. Kiayama, (Eds.). The heart’s eye: Emotional influences in perception and attention San Diego, CA Academic Press (pp. 167–196)

    Google Scholar 

  • Deruelle C., Rondan C., Gepner B., Tardif C., (2004). Spatial frequency and face processing in children with autism and Asperger syndrome Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 34:199–210

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Eskes G. A., Bryson S. E., McCormick T. A., (1990). Comprehension of concrete and abstract words in autistic children Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 20:61–73

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Fink G. R., Halligan, P. W., Marshall, J. C., Frith, C. D., Frackowiak, R. S., Dolan, R. J., (1997). Neural mechanisms involved in the processing of global and local aspects of hierarchically organized visual stimuli Brain 120:1779–1791

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Foxton J. M., Stewart M. E., Barnard L., Rodgers J., Young A. H., O’Brien G., Griffiths T. D., (2003). Absence of auditory ‘global interference’ in autism Brain 126:2703–2709

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Friston K. J., Buechel C., (2000). Attentional modulation of effective connectivity from V2 to V5/MT in humans Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 97:75912–75916

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith U., (1970). Studies in pattern detection in normal and autistic children: II. Reproduction and production of color sequences Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 10:120–135

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frith C., (2003). What do imaging studies tell us about the neural basis of autism? Novartis Foundation Symposium 251:149–166

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith C., (2004). Is autism a disconnection disorder? Lancet Neurology 3:577

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., (1989). Autism: explaining the Enigma. Blackwell

  • Frith U., Happé F., (1994). Autism: beyond “theory of mind” Cognition 50:115–132

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Frith U., Snowling M., (1983). Reading for meaning and reading for sound in autistic and dyslexic children Journal of Developmental Psychology 1:329–342

    Google Scholar 

  • Gasper K., Clore G. L., (2002). Attending to the big picture: Mood and global versus local processing of visual information Psychological Science 13:34–40

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gepner B., Mestre D. R., Masson G., de Schonen S., (1995). Postural effects of motion vision in young autistic children Neuroreport 6:1211–1214

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Gepner B., Mestre D. R., (2002). Brief report: Postural reactivity to fast visual motion differentiates autistic from children with Asperger syndrome Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders 32:231–238

    Google Scholar 

  • Gerland, G. (1997). A real person: Life on the outside [Translated from the Swedish by J. Tate]. Souvenir Press

  • Gustafsson L., (1997). Inadequate cortical feature maps: A neural circuit theory of autism Biological Psychiatry 42:1138–1147

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Happé F. G. E., (1994). Wechsler IQ profile and theory of mind in autism: A research note Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 35:1461–1471

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé F. G. E., (1996). Studying weak central coherence at low levels: Children with autism do not succumb to visual illusions, a research note Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 37:873–877

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé F. G. E., (1997). Central coherence and theory of mind in autism: Reading homographs in context British Journal of Developmental Psychology 15:1–12

    Google Scholar 

  • Happé F., (1999). Autism: Cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive Sciences 3:216–222

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé F., (2000). Parts and wholes, meaning and minds: Central coherence and its relation to theory of mind In: S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, D. Cohen, (eds.). Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism and developmental cognitive neuroscience Oxford Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Happé F., (2001). Social and non-social development in Autism: Where are the links? In: J. A. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, P. R. Zelazo, (eds.). Perspectives on development in autism NJ Lawrence Erlbaum Associates

    Google Scholar 

  • Happé F., (2003). Cognition in autism: One deficit or many? Autism: Neural basis and treatment possibilities(Novartis Foundation Symposium 251) Chichester Wiley pp. 198–212

    Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Booth, R. (in preparation). Sentence completion: a simple test of weak coherence in typical development, autism spectrum disorders and attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder

  • Happé F., Briskman J., Frith U., (2001). Exploring the cognitive phenotype of autism: Weak ‘central coherence’ in parents and siblings of children with autism. I. Experimental tests Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 42:299–307

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heaton P., (2003). Pitch memory, labelling and disembedding in autism Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 44:543–551

    Google Scholar 

  • Heaton P., Hermelin B., Pring L., (1998). Autism and pitch processing: a precursor for savant musical ability Music Perception 15:291–305

    Google Scholar 

  • Heavey L., (2003). Arithmetical savants In: Baroody A. J., Dowker A., (eds.). The development of arithmetic concepts and skills: Constructing adaptive expertise. Studies in mathematical thinking and learning Mahwah, NJ, USA Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers (pp. 409–433)

    Google Scholar 

  • Heavey L., Pring L., Hermelin B., (1999) A date to remember: The nature of memory in savant calendrical calculators Psychological Medicine 29:145–160

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Heinze H. J., Hinrichs H., Scholz M., Burchert W., Mangun G. R., (1998). Neural mechanisms of global and local processing: A combined PET and ERP study Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10:485–498

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hermelin B., O’Connor N., (1967). Remembering of words by psychotic and subnormal children British Journal of Psychology 58:213–218

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Hesse F. W., Spies K., (1996). Effects of negative mood on performance: Reduced capacity or changed processing strategy? European Journal of Social Psychology 26:163–168

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson R. P., Ouston J., Lee T., (1988). What’s in a face? The case of autism British Journal of Psychology 79:441–453

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jarrold C., Butler D. W., Cottington E. M., Jimenez F., (2000). Linking theory of mind and central coherence bias in autism and in the general population Developmental Psychology 36:126–138

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jarrold C., Russell J., (1997). Counting abilities in autism: Possible implications for central coherence theory Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 27:25–37

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe T., Baron-Cohen S., (1997). Are people with autism and Asperger syndrome faster than normal on the Embedded Figures Test? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 38:527–534

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe T., Baron-Cohen S., (1999). A test of central coherence theory; linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger’s syndrome: Is local coherence impaired? Cognition 71:149–185

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe T., Baron-Cohen S., (2000). Linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger's syndrome. Is global coherence impaired? Psychological Medicine 30:1169–1187

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Jolliffe T., Baron-Cohen S., (2001). A test of central coherence theory: Can adults with high-functioning autism or Asperger’s syndrome integrate fragments of an object? Cognitive Neuropsychiatry 6:193–216

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • John C. H., Hemsley D. R., (1992). Gestalt perception in schizophrenia European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience 241:215–221

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Just M. A., Cherkassky V. L., Keller T. A., Minshew N. J., (2004). Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: Evidence of underconnectivity Brain 127:1811–1821

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kanner L., (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact Nervous Child 2:217–250

    Google Scholar 

  • Klinger L. G., Dawson G., (2001). Prototype formation in autism Development & Psychopathology 13:111–124

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer J. H., Kaplan E., Blusewicz M. J., Preston K. A., (1991). Visual hierarchical analysis of block design configural errors Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology 13:455–465

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Kramer J. H., Ellenberg L., Leonard J., Share L. J., (1996). Developmental sex differences in global–local perceptual bias Neuropsychology 10:402–407

    Google Scholar 

  • Langdell T., (1978). Recognition of faces: An approach to the study of autism Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 19:255–268

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lawson J., Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S., (2004). Empathising and systemising in adults with and without Asperger syndrome Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders 34:301–310

    Google Scholar 

  • Lee T. S., (2002). Top–down influence in early visual processing: A Bayesian perspective Physiology and Behaviour 77:645–650

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Lopez B., Donnelly N., Hadwin J., Leekam S. R., (2004). Face processing in high-functioning adolescents with autism: Evidence for weak central coherence Visual Cognition 11:673–688

    Google Scholar 

  • Lopez B., Leekam S. R., (2003). Do children with autism fail to process information in context? Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 44:285–300

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann T. A., Walker P., (2003). Autism and a deficit in broadening the spread of visual attention Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 44:274–284

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marendaz C., (1985). Global precedence and field dependence: Visual routines? Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive 5:727–745

    Google Scholar 

  • McClelland J. L., (2000). The basis of hyperspecificity in autism: A preliminary suggestion based on properties of neural nets Journal of Autism & Developmental Disorders 30:497–502

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • McKelvey J. R., Lambert R., Mottron L., Shevell M. I., (1995). Right-hemisphere dysfunction in Asperger’s syndrome Journal of Child Neurology 10:310–314

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mevorach, C., Humphreys, G. W., & Shalev, L. (2005). Attending to local form while ignoring global aspects depends on handedness: evidence from TMS. Nature Neuroscience, published online doi: 10.1038/nn1400

  • Miller L. K., (1999). The savant syndrome: Intellectual impairment and exceptional skill Psychological Bulletin 125:31–46

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Milne, E. (2003). Investigating the correlates of weak central coherence in autism. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, University of London

  • Milne E., Swettenham J., Hansen P., Campbell R., Jeffries H., Plaisted K. I., (2002). High motion coherence thresholds in children with autism Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 43:255–263

    Google Scholar 

  • Morgan B., Maybery M., Durkin K., (2003). Weak central coherence, poor joint attention, and low verbal ability: Independent deficits in early autism Developmental Psychology 39:646–656

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron L., Belleville S., (1993). A study of perceptual analysis in a high-level autistic subject with exceptional graphic abilities Brain and Cognition 23:279–309

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron L., Belleville S., Ménard A., (1999). Local bias in autistic subjects as evidenced by graphic tasks: Perceptual hierarchization or working memory deficit? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 40:743–755

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Burack, J. A., Iarocci, G., Belleville, S., Enns, J. T. (2003). Locally oriented perception with intact global processing among adolescents with high-functioning autism: Evidence from multiple paradigms Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 40:904–913

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron L., Burack J. A., Stauder J. E. A., Robaey P., (1999). Perceptual processing among high-functioning persons with autism Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 40:203–211

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron L., Burack J. A., (2001). Enhanced perceptual functioning in the development of autism In: Burack J. A., Charman T., Yirmiya N., Zelazo P. R., (eds.). The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research NJ Lawrence Erlbaum

    Google Scholar 

  • Mottron L., Morasse K., Belleville S., (2001). A study of memory functioning in individuals with autism Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 42:253–260

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron L., Peretz I., Menard E., (2000). Local and global processing of music in high-functioning persons with autism: Beyond central coherence? Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 41:1057–1065

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Navon D., (1977). Forest before trees: The precedence of global features in visual perception Cognitive Psychology 9:353–383

    Google Scholar 

  • Navon D., (2003). What does a compound letter tell the psychologist’s mind? Acta Psychologica 114:273–309

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Norbury C. F., Bishop D. M. V., (2002). Inferential processing and story recall in children with communication problems: A comparison of specific language impairment, pragmatic language impairment and high-functioning autism International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders 37:227–251

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Riordan M. A., Plaisted K. C., Driver J., Baron-Cohen S., (2001). Superior visual search in autism Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance 27:719–730

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • O’Loughlin C., Thagard P., (2000). Autism and coherence: A computational model Mind & Language 15:375–392

    Google Scholar 

  • Ozonoff S., Strayer D. L., McMahon W. M., Filloux F., (1994). Executive function abilities in autism and Tourette syndrome: An information processing approach Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 35:1015–1032

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano E., Gibson L., Maybery M., Durkin K., Badcock D. R., (2005). Abnormal global processing along the dorsal visual pathway in autism: A possible mechanism for weak visuospatial coherence? Neuropsychologia 43:1044–1053

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, E., Maybery, M., & Durkin, K. (2005). Central coherence in typically developing preschoolers: Does it coherence and does it relate to mindreading and executive control? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 4:533–547

    Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted K. C., (2000). Aspects of autism that theory of mind cannot easily explain In: Baron-Cohen S., Tager-Flusberg H., Cohen D. J. (eds.). Understanding other minds: Perspectives from autism and cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed.). Oxford Oxford University Press

    Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted K. C., (2001). Reduced generalization in autism: An alternative to weak central coherence In: Burack J. A., Charman T., Yirmiya N., Zelazo P. R., (eds.). The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research New Jersey Lawrence Erlbaum pp. 149–169

    Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K. C., O’Riordan, M. A. F., Aitken, M. R. F., Killcross, A. S. (submitted for publication). Catergorisation in autism: Evidence for a reduced prototype effect

  • Plaisted K., O’Riordan M., Baron-Cohen S., (1998a). Enhanced visual search for a conjunctive target in autism: A research note Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 39:777–783

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted K., O’Riordan M., Baron-Cohen S., (1998b). Enhanced discrimination of novel, highly similar stimuli by adults with autism during a perceptual learning task Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 39:765–775

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted K., Saksida L., Alcantara J., Weisblatt E., (2003). Towards an understanding of the mechanisms of weak central coherence effects: Experiments in visual configural learning and auditory perception Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London—Series B: Biological Sciences 358:375–386

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted K., Swettenham J., Rees L., (1999). Children with autism show local precedence in a divided attention task and global precedence in a selective attention task Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 40:733–742

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Pring L. Hermelin B., (1993). Bottle, tulip and wineglass: Semantic and structural picture processing by savant artists Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 34:1365–1385

    Google Scholar 

  • Pring L., Hermelin B., Heavey L., (1995). Savants, segments, art and autism Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 36:1065–1076

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rincover A., Koegel R. L., (1975). Setting generality and stimulus control in autistic children Journal of Applied Behaviour Analysis 8:235–246

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rinehart N. J., Bradshaw J. L., Moss S. A., Brereton A. V., Tonge B. J., (2000). Atypical interference of local detail on global processing in high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 41:769–778

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rinehart N. J., Bradshaw J. L., Moss S. A., Brereton A. V., Tonge B. J. (2001). A deficit in shifting attention present in high-functioning autism but not Asperger’s disorder Autism 5:67–80

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ring H. A., Baron-Cohen S., Wheelwright S., Williams S. C., Brammer M., Andrew C., Bullmore E. T., (1999). Cerebral correlates of preserved cognitive skills in autism: A functional MRI study of embedded figures task performance Brain 122:1305–1315

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson L. C., Lamb M. R., (1991). Neuropsychological contribution to theories of part/whole organization Cognitive Psychology 23:299–330

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ronald, A., Happé, F., & Plomin, R. (2005). The genetic relationship between individual differences in social and nonsocial behaviours characteristic of autism. Developmental Science 8: 444–458

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ropar D., Mitchell P., (1999). Are individuals with autism and Asperger’s syndrome susceptible to visual illusions? Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 40:1283–1293

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Ropar D., Mitchell P., (2001). Susceptibility to illusions and performance on visuospatial tasks in individuals with autism Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 42:539–549

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Rouse H., Donnelly N., Hadwin J. A., Brown T., (2004). Do children with autism perceive second-order relational features? The case of the Thatcher illusion Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry 45:1246–1257

    Google Scholar 

  • Scheuffgen, K. (1998). Domain-general and domain-specific deficits in autism and dyslexia. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, University of London

  • Scott, F. J., Brosnan, M. J., & Wheelwright, S. (submitted for publication). Perception of illusions by people with autism: Is there a low-level central coherence deficit?

  • Shah A., Frith U., (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: A research note Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 24:613–620

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Shah A., Frith U., (1993). Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the Block Design task? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 34:1351–1364

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling M., Frith U., (1986). Comprehension in ‘hyperlexic’ readers Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 42:392–415

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer J., O’Brien J., Riggs K., Braddick O., Atkinson J., Wattam-Bell J., (2000). Motion processing in autism: Evidence for a dorsal stream deficiency Neuroreport 11:2765–2767

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stiles-Davis J., Janowsky J., Engel M., Nass R., (1988). Drawing ability in four young children with congenital unilateral brain lesions Neuropsychologia 26:359–371

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Stoecker J. J., Colombo J., Frick J. E., Allen J. R., (1998). Long- and short-looking infants’ recognition of symmetrical and asymmetrical forms Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 71:63–78

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Suzuki S., Cavanagh P., (1995). Facial organization blocks access to low-level features: An object inferiority effect Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and Performance 21:901–913

    Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg H., (1991). Semantic processing in the free recall of autistic children: Further evidence for a cognitive deficit British Journal of Developmental Psychology 9:417–430

    Google Scholar 

  • Teunisse J.-P., Cools A. R., van Spaendonck K. P. M., Aerts F. H., Berger H. J. (2001). Cognitive styles in high-functioning adolescents with autistic disorder Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 31:55–66

    PubMed  CAS  Google Scholar 

  • Teunisse J. P., de Gelder B., (2003). Face processing in adolescents with autistic disorder: The inversion and composite effects Brain & Cognition 52:285–294

    Google Scholar 

  • Uhlhaas P. J., Silverstein S. M., Phillips W. A., Lovell P. G., (2004). Evidence for impaired visual context processing in schizotypy with thought disorder Schizophrenia Research 68:249–260

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Volberg G., Hubner R., (2004). On the role of response conflicts and stimulus position for hemispheric differences in global/local processing: An ERP study Neuropsychologia 42:1805–1813

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Waiter G. D., Williams J. H., Murray A. D., Gilchrist A., Perrett D. I., Whiten A., (2005). Structural white matter deficits in high-functioning individuals with autistic spectrum disorder: A voxel-based investigation Neuroimage 24:455–461

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Witkin H. A., Dyk R. B., Faterson H. F., Goodenough D. R., Karp S. K., (1962). Psychological differentiation New York Wiley

    Google Scholar 

  • Witkin H. A., Goodenough D. R., (1981). Cognitive styles: Essence and origins New York International University Press

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Both authors were supported by grants from the Medical Research Council at the time of writing (grant G9900087 to F.H. and G9716841 to U.F.). We would like to thank Rhonda Booth for her helpful comments on the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Francesca Happé.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Happé, F., Frith, U. The Weak Coherence Account: Detail-focused Cognitive Style in Autism Spectrum Disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 36, 5–25 (2006). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0

Keywords

Navigation