Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Current Issue
  • Ahead of Print
  • Past Issues
  • Info for
    • Authors
    • Print Subscriptions
  • About
    • About the Journal
    • About the Academy
    • Editorial Board
  • Feedback
  • Alerts
  • AAPL

User menu

  • Alerts
  • Log out

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law
  • AAPL
  • Alerts
  • Log out
Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Current Issue
  • Ahead of Print
  • Past Issues
  • Info for
    • Authors
    • Print Subscriptions
  • About
    • About the Journal
    • About the Academy
    • Editorial Board
  • Feedback
  • Alerts
OtherREGULAR ARTICLE

Commentary: The Future of Forensic Functional Brain Imaging

Daniel D. Langleben and Frank M. Dattilio
Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online December 2008, 36 (4) 502-504;
Daniel D. Langleben
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Frank M. Dattilio
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

In “Functional MRI Lie Detection: Too Good to be True?” in this issue of The Journal, Joseph Simpson reviews the merits and the limitations of using fMRI to detect deception. After presenting the gaps in experimental data that stand in the way of translating the laboratory proof of concept to a field application, Simpson surveys the legal, regulatory and ethics concerns facing fMRI, should it emerge as a technologically robust method of lie detection. In our commentary, we update and interpret the data described by Simpson, from the points of view of an experimental scientist and a forensic clinician. We conclude that the current research funding and literature are prematurely skewed toward discussion of existing findings, rather than generation of new fMRI data on deception and related topics such as mind‐reading, consciousness, morality, and criminal responsibility. We propose that further progress in brain imaging research may foster the emergence of a new discipline of forensic MRI.

In his article, “Functional MRI Lie Detection: Too Good to be True?,”1 Joseph Simpson reviews and comments on the emerging field of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)‐based lie detection. In the past few years, experts from many relevant disciplines have written reviews,2–14 journal articles,15,16 and books17–19 that set accuracy and originality standards that are a challenge for the new entrants.

Rome did not fall because it was weak; it fell because it became weaker than the barbarians at its gates. The polygraph did not become more or less accurate in the past decade. Rather, it failed to keep up with the neuroscientists. There were warnings. In 1988, the year of the Federal Employment Polygraph Protection Act (FEPPA), Rosenfeld20 reported using the P‐300 component of the electroencephalogram (EEG) to detect deception. A few years later, Farwell21–23 commercialized the technology. Farwell is still in business (Brain Fingerprinting Laboratories, Inc., Seattle, WA). Simpson repeats24 the important question: should fMRI be regulated under the FPPA? The reader would benefit from an in‐depth look into this matter. Both fMRI and EEG are measures of central nervous system activity that have been proposed as an alternative to polygraph. Neither has been shown to be clinically valid. Is regulating fMRI, but not EEG, under the FEPPA, reasonable and defensible? Are the images generated by fMRI particularly dangerous to the impressionable juries and human resource directors? Probably not, since the new 128‐lead EEG systems could be used to generate equally impressive images of brain activity. Or is this special treatment a badge of recognition of fMRI's true potential?

Simpson's survey omits many recent contributions to the field.5–8,10,25–27 Of particular interest is work by Abe et al.28 on the activity of the limbic system during deception. Considering the role of emotion in polygraphy, the absence of limbic activation from most reported fMRI patterns of deception is intriguing and may be construed as a proof of the fundamental difference between the peripheral and central nervous system correlates of deception.6 Equally important to consider is the continuous progress in fMRI research of executive functions, memory, and real‐time fMRI that is critical to the future of applied forensic fMRI, including lie detection.29–32 In his review, Simpson does not distinguish between the researchers of deception and the merchants of fMRI‐based lie detection. While the overwhelming majority of the former are recognized scientists, it would be interesting to know more about the latter. The uneasy alliances between this industry and academia, brokered by university technology commercialization departments,16 are yet another topic that we wish Simpson had shed some light on.

Regarding Simpson's assumption that “…there do not appear to have been any instances of the use of fMRI lie detection in a legal or employment setting” (Ref. 1, p 492) we note that Harvey Nathan was tested by No Lie MRI, Inc. in an insurance fraud case in 2007. A brief review of an ABC television description of Mr. Nathan's case33 suggests that the goal of Mr. Nathan's investigation was to prove his innocence, rather than guilt. In all published fMRI lie‐detection studies, presence of deception was guaranteed by experimental design. Was the test, given to Mr. Nathan by No Lie MRI, Inc., appropriate for a situation in which deception could be absent? Mr. Nathan's case highlights several interesting points we wish Simpson had elaborated on. First, does absence of deception prove innocence? That would depend on the tests' false negative rate.3 Second, is the same combination of specificity, sensitivity, and prevalence appropriate for civil, criminal, and nonlegal (e.g., insurance) cases? Specificity that is not high enough to prove innocence positively in a civil case may still be sufficient to raise reasonable doubt in a criminal case. Defending Mr. Nathan's fMRI study in court would require normative data that, to the best of our knowledge, is not yet available in the peer‐reviewed literature. Without such data, truth verification with fMRI could degenerate into a brisk trade in false‐negative fMRI findings. In his last neuroscience‐related paragraph, Simpson may have confounded two fundamentally different applications of fMRI for information gathering: mind reading and lie detection.5,34 While lie detection implies a query and a deceptive response, mind‐reading requires neither. Mind‐reading with fMRI is scientifically more challenging and even less developed than lie detection, yet it is of great interest and relevance to forensic psychiatry. Despite the multitude of technical challenges, we believe that fMRI is a powerful technology that may, together with the more established structural MRI, form a discipline of forensic MRI in the foreseeable future.

Simpson quotes an ethicist's concern that “At the most basic level is the question of whether a precise definition of lying even exists” (Ref. 1, p 495). The answer to this is; “Yes, they do exist.” As there are multiple types of lying,35 so are there multiple definitions, relevant to a particular domain of culture, science, or law.36–39 It is sufficient for biological researchers of deception to commit, a priori, to a specific definition of deception and incorporate it in the model they use to produce deception in their experiments.3,39 Whether the model of deception used in a particular research study or a case is externally valid is up to the peer review process or the courts.

Simpson's article provides a comprehensive list of the critical knowledge gaps that include the effects of countermeasures, motivation, and demographics on the relevance of fMRI to real‐world deception. For an experimental scientist, items on this list have one common thread: studies that would fill these gaps will require larger samples than those provided in any prior fMRI study of deception. Yes, the gaps between a proof of concept, a prototype, and a fieldworthy test have to be filled with research subjects, and thus with much more funding. The fMRI lie‐detection technology is at a stage of development similar to a Phase 2 Food and Drug Administration registration trial of a new medication. Phase 3 trials are now due. In the pharmaceutical industry, Phase 3 trials cost hundreds of millions of dollars. Commercial lie‐detection interests have neither the technical nor the financial muscle of their pharmaceutical colleagues. The total private investment in fMRI‐based lie detection research so far can be estimated to be a few million U.S. dollars. The official government investment is even smaller. Larger sums may have been spent in relative obscurity by groups closely affiliated with the Department of Defense. One example is a $5,000,000 U.S. Congress earmark in the 2004 and 2005 Defense spending bill to fund research on “advanced technologies for deception detection” at the University of South Carolina at Columbia, SC.40 Such a method of funding research has been criticized,17 because of the potential conflicts of interest.

Despite the relative dearth of fMRI and other biological data on deception, there is a proliferation of scholarly reviews on the subject that threatens to suffocate this emerging field of study. Extending this imbalance, the funding for the elaboration of the ethics and legal dimensions of neuroscience‐based lie detection equals or exceeds that provided to acquire new data. An example is McArthur Foundation's $10,000,000 Law and Neuroscience Project,41 seemingly dedicated to discussion rather than generation of pertinent experimental data. We believe that independent, competitive, and distributed research funding, similar to the one administered by the National Institutes of Health, is necessary to generate the independent data we need to tell whether lie detection with fMRI is “too good to be true.”

  • American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law

References

  1. ↵
    Simpson JR: Functional MRI lie detection: too good to be true? J Am Acad Psychiatry Law 36:491–8, 2008
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  2. ↵
    Spence SA: The deceptive brain. J R Soc Med 97:6–9, 2004
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  3. ↵
    Wolpe PR, Foster KR, Langleben DD: Emerging neurotechnologies for lie‐detection: promises and perils. Am J Bioeth 5:39–49, 2005
    OpenUrlPubMed
  4. Appelbaum PS: Law and psychiatry: the new lie detectors—neuroscience, deception, and the courts. Psychiatr Serv 58:460–2, 2007
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  5. ↵
    Haynes JD, Sakai K, Rees G, et al: Reading hidden intentions in the human brain. Curr Biol 17:323–8, 2007
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. ↵
    Hakun JG, Ruparel K, Seelig D, et al: Towards clinical trials of lie detection with fMRI. Soc Neurosci 12:1–10, 2008
    OpenUrl
  7. Haynes JD: Detecting deception from neuroimaging signals: a data‐driven perspective. Trends Cogn Sci 12:126–7, 2008
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. ↵
    Langleben DD: Detection of deception with fMRI: are we there yet? Legal Criminol Psychol 13:1–9, 2008
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  9. Sip KE, Roepstorff A, McGregor W, et al: Detecting deception: the scope and limits. Trends Cogn Sci 12:48–53, 2008
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. ↵
    Spence SA: Playing devil's advocate: the case against fMRI lie detection. Legal Criminol Psychol 13:11–25, 2008
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  11. Happel MD: Neuroscience and the detection of deception. Rev Policy Res 22:667–85, 2005
    OpenUrl
  12. Canli T, Brandon S, Casebeer W, et al: Neuroethics and national security. Am J Bioeth 7:3–13, 2007
    OpenUrlPubMed
  13. Johnson KA, Kozel FA, Laken SJ, et al: The neuroscience of functional magnetic resonance imaging fMRI for deception detection. Am J Bioeth 7:58–60, 2007
    OpenUrlPubMed
  14. ↵
    Thompson SK: The legality of the use of psychiatric neuroimaging in intelligence interrogation. Cornell Law Rev 90:1601–37, 2005
    OpenUrlPubMed
  15. ↵
    Talbot M: Duped: can brain scans uncover lies? The New Yorker. July 2, 2007, pp 52–61
  16. ↵
    Wolpe P, Langleben D: Lies, damn lies, and lie detectors: breakthrough ideas for 2008. Harvard Bus Rev 86:1, 2008
    OpenUrl
  17. ↵
    Stern PC (editor): The Polygraph and Lie Detection. Report of The National Research Council Committee to Review the Scientific Evidence on the Polygraph. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press, 2004
  18. National Research Council: Emerging Cognitive Neuroscience and Related Technologies. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press, 2008
  19. ↵
    Moreno JD: Mind Wars: Brain Research and National Defense. Chicago, IL: Dana Press, 2006
  20. ↵
    Rosenfeld JP, Cantwell B, Nasman VT, et al: A modified, event‐related potential‐based guilty knowledge test. Int J Neurosci 42:157–61, 1988
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  21. ↵
    Farwell LA: Method and apparatus for detection of deception. U.S. Patent and Trademark Office. Westport, CT: University Patents, Inc., 1990
  22. Farwell LA: Method for electroencephalographic information detection. U.S. Patent and Trademark Office. Swampscott, MA: Conte, Francis Luca, 1991
  23. ↵
    Farwell LA, Donchin E: The truth will out: interrogative polygraphy with event‐related brain potentials. Psychophysiology 28:531–47, 1991
    OpenUrlPubMed
  24. ↵
    Tovino SA: Functional neuroimaging and the law: trends and directions for future scholarship. Am J Bioeth 7:44–56, 2007
    OpenUrlPubMed
  25. ↵
    Kozel FA, Trivedi MH: Developing a neuropsychiatric functional brain imaging test. Neurocase 15:1–5, 2008
    OpenUrlPubMed
  26. Hakun JG, Seelig D, Ruparel K, et al: fMRI investigation of the cognitive structure of the Concealed Information Test. Neurocase 14:59–67, 2008
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  27. ↵
    Gamer M, Bauermann T, Stoeter P, et al: Covariations among fMRI, skin conductance, and behavioral data during processing of concealed information. Hum Brain Mapp 28:1287–301, 2007
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  28. ↵
    Abe N, Okuda J, Suzuki M, et al: Neural correlates of true memory, false memory, and deception. Cereb Cortex, published online March 27, 2008
  29. ↵
    Anderson MC, Ochsner KN, Kuhl B, et al: Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories. Science 303:232–5, 2004
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  30. Aron AR: The neural basis of inhibition in cognitive control. Neuroscientist 13:214–28, 2007
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  31. Posse S, Binkofski F, Schneider F, et al: A new approach to measure single‐event related brain activity using real‐time fMRI: feasibility of sensory, motor, and higher cognitive tasks. Hum Brain Mapp 12:25–41, 2001
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  32. ↵
    Spence SA: Prefrontal white matter: the tissue of lies?—invited commentary on “prefrontal white matter in pathological liars.” Br J Psychiatry 187:326–7, 2005
    OpenUrlFREE Full Text
  33. ↵
    http://abclocal.go.com/wpvi/story?section=news/technology&id=5621892/ Accessed September 8, 2008
  34. ↵
    Norman KA, Polyn SM, Detre GJ, et al: Beyond mind‐reading: multi‐voxel pattern analysis of fMRI data. Trends Cogn Sci 10:424–30, 2006
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. ↵
    Ganis G, Kosslyn SM, Stose S, et al: Neural correlates of different types of deception: an FMRI investigation. Cereb Cortex 13:830–6, 2003
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  36. ↵
    Vrij A: Detecting Lies and Deceit: The Psychology of Lying and the Implications for Professional Practice. Chichester, UK: Wiley, 2001
  37. Saint Augustine, Bishop of Hippo: On lying, in On the Holy Trinity; Doctrinal Treatises; Moral Treatises (A Select Library of the Christian Church: Nicene Fathers; First Series, vol 3). Edited by Schaff P. Peabody, MA: Hendrickson Publishers, 1994, pp 457–77
  38. Spence SA, Hunter MD, Farrow TF, et al: A cognitive neurobiological account of deception: evidence from functional neuroimaging. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 359:1755–62, 2004
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  39. ↵
    Langleben DD, Schroeder L, Maldjian JA, et al: Brain activity during simulated deception: an event‐related functional magnetic resonance study. Neuroimage 15:727–32, 2002
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  40. ↵
    Hickman WB: Senate Approves $131 Million for South Carolina Defense Projects. Edited by Graham SL. 2005. Available at http://lgraham.senate.gov/public/index.cfm?FuseAction=PressRoom.PressReleases&ContentRecord_id=c771894b-4eb5-4304-98f0-3bf5ddc3b78c&Region_id=&Issue_id=. Accessed November 1, 2008
  41. ↵
    http://www.lawandneuroscienceproject.org/. Accessed November 1, 2008
PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online: 36 (4)
Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online
Vol. 36, Issue 4
December 2008
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Print
Download PDF
Article Alerts
Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in recommending The Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law site.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Commentary: The Future of Forensic Functional Brain Imaging
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law
(Your Name) thought you would like to see this page from the Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Commentary: The Future of Forensic Functional Brain Imaging
Daniel D. Langleben, Frank M. Dattilio
Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online Dec 2008, 36 (4) 502-504;

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero

Share
Commentary: The Future of Forensic Functional Brain Imaging
Daniel D. Langleben, Frank M. Dattilio
Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online Dec 2008, 36 (4) 502-504;
del.icio.us logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • References
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • A Forensic Science-Based Model for Identifying and Mitigating Forensic Mental Health Expert Biases
  • Bias in Peer Review of Forensic Psychiatry Publications
  • Reconsidering the Relationship Between Criminal Insanity and Delusions
Show more Regular Article

Similar Articles

Site Navigation

  • Home
  • Current Issue
  • Ahead of Print
  • Archive
  • Information for Authors
  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Feedback
  • Alerts

Other Resources

  • Academy Website
  • AAPL Meetings
  • AAPL Annual Review Course

Reviewers

  • Peer Reviewers

Other Publications

  • AAPL Practice Guidelines
  • AAPL Newsletter
  • AAPL Ethics Guidelines
  • AAPL Amicus Briefs
  • Landmark Cases

Customer Service

  • Cookie Policy
  • Reprints and Permissions
  • Order Physical Copy

Copyright © 2025 by The American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law