Skip to main content

Main menu

  • Home
  • Current Issue
  • Ahead of Print
  • Past Issues
  • Info for
    • Authors
    • Print Subscriptions
  • About
    • About the Journal
    • About the Academy
    • Editorial Board
  • Feedback
  • Alerts
  • AAPL

User menu

  • Alerts

Search

  • Advanced search
Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law
  • AAPL
  • Alerts
Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law

Advanced Search

  • Home
  • Current Issue
  • Ahead of Print
  • Past Issues
  • Info for
    • Authors
    • Print Subscriptions
  • About
    • About the Journal
    • About the Academy
    • Editorial Board
  • Feedback
  • Alerts
Research ArticleRegular Articles

Clinical, Social, and Neuropsychological Dimensions of the Intersection of Addiction and Criminality

Paul G. Nestor, Ashley Woodhull, Dominick Newell, Keira O'Donovan, Mayte Forte, Sean Harding and Marc Pomplun
Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online June 2018, 46 (2) 179-186; DOI: https://doi.org/10.29158/JAAPL.003745-18
Paul G. Nestor
Dr. Nestor is Professor of Psychology, University of Massachusetts, Boston, MA, and Lecturer in Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA. Dr. Woodhull is a Post-Doctoral Fellow, Edith Nourse Rogers Memorial Veterans Hospital, Bedford, MA. Mr. Newell is a Community Residence Counselor, McLean Hospital, Boston, MA. Ms. O'Donovan and Ms. Forte are Clinical Psychology Graduate Students and Dr. Pomplun is Professor of Computer Science, University of Massachusetts, Boston, MA. Mr. Harding is Director of Clinical Services, SPAN Inc., Boston, MA.
PhD
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Ashley Woodhull
Dr. Nestor is Professor of Psychology, University of Massachusetts, Boston, MA, and Lecturer in Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA. Dr. Woodhull is a Post-Doctoral Fellow, Edith Nourse Rogers Memorial Veterans Hospital, Bedford, MA. Mr. Newell is a Community Residence Counselor, McLean Hospital, Boston, MA. Ms. O'Donovan and Ms. Forte are Clinical Psychology Graduate Students and Dr. Pomplun is Professor of Computer Science, University of Massachusetts, Boston, MA. Mr. Harding is Director of Clinical Services, SPAN Inc., Boston, MA.
PhD
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Dominick Newell
Dr. Nestor is Professor of Psychology, University of Massachusetts, Boston, MA, and Lecturer in Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA. Dr. Woodhull is a Post-Doctoral Fellow, Edith Nourse Rogers Memorial Veterans Hospital, Bedford, MA. Mr. Newell is a Community Residence Counselor, McLean Hospital, Boston, MA. Ms. O'Donovan and Ms. Forte are Clinical Psychology Graduate Students and Dr. Pomplun is Professor of Computer Science, University of Massachusetts, Boston, MA. Mr. Harding is Director of Clinical Services, SPAN Inc., Boston, MA.
BA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Keira O'Donovan
Dr. Nestor is Professor of Psychology, University of Massachusetts, Boston, MA, and Lecturer in Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA. Dr. Woodhull is a Post-Doctoral Fellow, Edith Nourse Rogers Memorial Veterans Hospital, Bedford, MA. Mr. Newell is a Community Residence Counselor, McLean Hospital, Boston, MA. Ms. O'Donovan and Ms. Forte are Clinical Psychology Graduate Students and Dr. Pomplun is Professor of Computer Science, University of Massachusetts, Boston, MA. Mr. Harding is Director of Clinical Services, SPAN Inc., Boston, MA.
BA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Mayte Forte
Dr. Nestor is Professor of Psychology, University of Massachusetts, Boston, MA, and Lecturer in Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA. Dr. Woodhull is a Post-Doctoral Fellow, Edith Nourse Rogers Memorial Veterans Hospital, Bedford, MA. Mr. Newell is a Community Residence Counselor, McLean Hospital, Boston, MA. Ms. O'Donovan and Ms. Forte are Clinical Psychology Graduate Students and Dr. Pomplun is Professor of Computer Science, University of Massachusetts, Boston, MA. Mr. Harding is Director of Clinical Services, SPAN Inc., Boston, MA.
BA
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Sean Harding
Dr. Nestor is Professor of Psychology, University of Massachusetts, Boston, MA, and Lecturer in Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA. Dr. Woodhull is a Post-Doctoral Fellow, Edith Nourse Rogers Memorial Veterans Hospital, Bedford, MA. Mr. Newell is a Community Residence Counselor, McLean Hospital, Boston, MA. Ms. O'Donovan and Ms. Forte are Clinical Psychology Graduate Students and Dr. Pomplun is Professor of Computer Science, University of Massachusetts, Boston, MA. Mr. Harding is Director of Clinical Services, SPAN Inc., Boston, MA.
MSW
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
Marc Pomplun
Dr. Nestor is Professor of Psychology, University of Massachusetts, Boston, MA, and Lecturer in Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA. Dr. Woodhull is a Post-Doctoral Fellow, Edith Nourse Rogers Memorial Veterans Hospital, Bedford, MA. Mr. Newell is a Community Residence Counselor, McLean Hospital, Boston, MA. Ms. O'Donovan and Ms. Forte are Clinical Psychology Graduate Students and Dr. Pomplun is Professor of Computer Science, University of Massachusetts, Boston, MA. Mr. Harding is Director of Clinical Services, SPAN Inc., Boston, MA.
PhD
  • Find this author on Google Scholar
  • Find this author on PubMed
  • Search for this author on this site
  • Article
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF
Loading

Abstract

To examine the social cognitive processes underlying the relationship of addiction and criminality, we administered the Addiction Severity Index-CF (ASI-CF) and the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) to 35 participants (11 women) who had been released recently from jail or prison. ASI-CF revealed highest lifetime drug use for alcohol, followed by cannabis, cocaine, and heroin, with an average of 42 lifetime arrests in this ex-offender sample. The men and the women showed similar psychiatric histories marked by depression and anxiety, but the women had more lifetime problems with thinking than did the men, who had higher lifetime problems with hallucinations. On the IGT, participants showed evidence of reward-learning across the initial three blocks of 20 trials, but their performance declined over the last 40 trials, suggesting a failure to sustain gains: that is, to learn from feedback in deciding among advantageous and disadvantageous decks of cards. These deficits in motivated decision-making were moderated by current social and psychological factors, as assessed by ASI-CF. These results are discussed in regard to how disturbances in social cognitive processes may underlie the relationship of addiction and criminality.

Lifetime substance abuse disorders are estimated to occur in 17 percent of the general population.1 For criminal offenders, these rates are exponentially higher, with substance abuse disorders occurring at more than four times the rate in the general population. In addition, approximately 75 percent of prison inmates are estimated to meet diagnostic criteria for co-occurring substance abuse disorder and mental illness.2

In light of the persistent and troubling link of addiction and criminality, recent efforts have been directed toward implementing randomized control trials to examine the efficacy of treatment in managing and mitigating risk and reducing recidivism in male ex-offenders with substance abuse.3,–,6 These studies have shown promising results over the short term; for example, Gordon et al.3 demonstrated the efficacy of combined methadone and counseling treatment in reducing self-reported heroin use and criminal activity over six months after release in male inmates who were heroin dependent during the year before incarceration. However, these same studies failed to demonstrate the expected treatment effect in reducing recidivism beyond six months after incarceration (at 12-month follow-up).3,–,6 Thus, as these studies clearly demonstrate, the factors maintaining the cycle of substance abuse and criminality remain an active area of research.

Accordingly, in the current study, we examined some of the key dimensions that characterize the relationship of addiction and criminality, with the rationale being that such information may help in developing more effective treatment. This aim aligns well with recent advances in the study of mental health that have emphasized the importance of examining core dimensions of psychopathology rather than discrete disorder categories.7,–,10 Four core dimensions (affective, cognitive, social, and motivational) and their constituent neural processes are frequently the focus of what is often referred to as a transdiagnostic approach to the study of diverse forms of psychopathology.11 These dimensions have, in turn, been studied from the broad perspective of social cognition.12 Here, social cognitive perspectives view substance abuse and criminality as the product of the dynamic interplay of complex social phenomena and their accompanying psychological and neural mechanisms.13 In so doing, social cognition provides a conceptual framework to investigate individual differences in substance abuse disordered ex-offenders with respect to affective, motivational, cognitive and social processes that may factor prominently in their maladaptive behavior and impaired decision-making, as well as in their treatment failure.

In the current study, we therefore examined retrospective substance abuse and criminality through the lens of social cognition in a sample of ex-offenders recently released from jail or prison. We also compared male and female ex-offenders as the effects of gender on substance abuse and criminality remains an understudied area of research. To this end, we assessed male and female ex-offenders with the Addiction Severity Index-CF (ASI-CF), a semistructured interview that covers areas relevant to both substance abuse and criminality,14 such as items related to emotion, cognition, behavior, and psychosis and those related to social functioning, such as time spent alone, interpersonal conflict, and family relationships. The ASI-CF also includes items that may be viewed as tapping motivation, as for examples questions regarding satisfaction with use of free time and concerns about legal problems.

In the current study, the ASI was complemented by a widely used computerized neuropsychological measure of incremental learning and decision-making, the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), which is thought to be particularly sensitive to the deleterious effects of substance abuse on the reward circuitry of the brain15: specifically, dopaminergic rich, cortical–striatal connections. Moreover, recent neurophysiologic and neuropharmacological data from healthy adults16 have shown N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptor-supported long-term potentiation (LTP) at cortical–striatal synapses plays a critical role in the cellular machinery of learning stimulus–outcome contingencies of the type that are assessed by the IGT. In clinical samples, IGT has been shown to be sensitive to neuropsychological disturbances, not only in substance abuse populations17,18 but also in other clinical disorders,19 including schizophrenia,20,21 psychopathy,22 and ventromedial prefrontal cortex damage.15

In fact, we recently examined the IGT and psychopathy in a sample of male ex-offenders who had been released from jail or prison and were receiving services from the same reentry agency from which the participants in the current study were drawn. The results pointed to the close relationship of reduced IGT performance and higher levels of psychopathy, as measured by objective ratings. In addition, both IGT performance and psychopathy ratings predicted recidivism. Perhaps most notably, hierarchical multiple regression results showed IGT to be a stronger predictor of recidivism than psychopathy rating scales in this sample of male offenders.22 We now combined ASI-CF results with those obtained with IGT to examine the relationship of addiction and criminality in female and male ex-offenders.

Method

Sample

Thirty-five participants (24 men, 11 women) had a mean (SD) age of 43.04 (2.02) years and a mean level of education of 12.63 (10.55) years. Male participants had a mean age of 44.75 (9.26) years and a mean level of education of 12.17 (1.97) years in comparison with the mean age of 39.14 (2.99) years and education of 13.64 (1.80) years of female participants. Of the ex-offender group, 33 percent identified as black/non-Hispanic, 29 percent as Hispanic, 29 percent as white, and 9 percent as other.

Addiction Severity Index-Lite CF

The Addiction Severity Index (ASI) is a semistructured interview first developed more than 30 years ago14 and widely used in both research and clinical settings in the assessment of substance abuse disorders.23 Translated into several languages, the ASI has been demonstrated to provide a reliable and valid assessment of the nature and course of substance abuse disorders and its related conditions.24,–,28 The interviewer assesses self-reported problems in seven areas of functioning: physical health, employment and financial support, illegal or criminal activity, family and social relationships, psychiatric symptoms, and drug and alcohol use. For each problem area, current (within the past 30 days) and lifetime experience are tabulated. For the current study, we used the ASI-Lite CF, which covers the same seven areas as the full ASI, but with fewer items.29 Psychometric studies have shown that the ASI-Lite CF offers a reliable and valid analog to the full ASI.29

Iowa Gambling Task

On the computerized version of the IGT,30 a test of decision-making under risk, subjects are instructed to maximize monetary payoffs by making a series of choices via selecting cards from one of four decks. At the start of the task, subjects are told to select cards to try to maximize their winnings, continuing until signaled by the computer to stop. Unbeknownst to the subjects, there are 100 trials. Subjects view four decks of cards, labeled A, B, C, and D, as well as the running tab of their total earnings. With each selection, the card turns face up to show the winning and loss amounts, which vary between and within decks. Decks A and B are overall disadvantageous, as card selections produce larger wins, but even greater losses, resulting in a net loss. Decks C and D are overall advantageous, for which little money is won on a single choice but even less is lost, resulting in a net gain. IGT performance is summarized by number of advantageous and disadvantageous selections. These are reported as a total net score (advantageous minus disadvantageous). In addition, to examine the effects of experience on task, performance is divided into five blocks of 20 trials, and a net score is calculated for each of the five blocks. Higher net scores on the IGT reflect more advantageous decision-making performance. IGT scores were available for 34 participants.

Procedure

All participants had been recently released from incarceration and were recruited from an offender reentry service program, Span, Inc., located in downtown Boston, MA. All participants first provided written informed consent. Then, the ASI-CF interview and neuropsychological tests (IGT and the oral reading subtest of the Wide Range Achievement Test Fourth Edition (WRAT-4)31) were administered. Testing and interview took approximately 90 minutes and all participants were paid for their time in the form of gift cards. Testing and interviews were conducted by graduate and undergraduate students at the University of Massachusetts Boston (UMB) under the supervision of a licensed psychologist (P.G.N.). The UMB Institutional Review Board (IRB) approved the study.

Results

Drug History

As shown in Table 1, participants reported highest lifetime drug use in mean (SD) years for alcohol followed by cannabis, cocaine, and heroin. Lifetime drug use did not differ in the male and female participants. In addition, lifetime alcohol use correlated with cocaine use (r = 0.416), as did cannabis with heroin (r = 0.443; p = .008). Lifetime use of sedative hypnotics also correlated strongly with lifetime use of amphetamines (r = 0.579; p < .001) and hallucinogens (r = 0.645; p < .001). For men only, the data revealed similar significant lifetime drug use correlations as those found in the entire sample, with the exception that sedative hypnotics correlated strongly with lifetime barbiturate (r = 0.993; p < .001), but not with lifetime amphetamine use. For women, the drug correlations that were also found in the entire sample occurred for sedative hypnotics with amphetamines (r = 0.781; p = .013) and with hallucinogens (r = 0.869; p = .001); lifetime use of amphetamines also correlated very strongly with hallucinogens (r = 0.965; p < .001).

View this table:
  • View inline
  • View popup
Table 1

Current and Past Lifetime Drug Use

Criminal History

Over their lifetimes, participants spent a mean (SD) of 136.91 (135.52) months incarcerated for approximately 11.4 years, with a mean lifetime number of arrests of 42.44 (55.76). The highest reported mean number of arrests were for burglary, larceny, or breaking-and-entering (4.74 (10.96)), followed by parole or probation violation (3.69 (6.28)), drug charges (3.26 (3.62)), assault (2.74 (4.60)), motor vehicle violations (2.46 (6.83)), and shoplifting (2.37, 4.66). Female participants reported fewer overall lifetime arrests (26.00 (17.92)) than did male participants (49.62 (65.20)), as well as fewer arrests across the various criminal charges, but the differences were not statistically significant.

In response to the question, “How serious do you feel your present legal problems are?” 22 of 34 participants answered “not at all” and another three participants answered “slightly.” Thus, 73.5 percent of the sample responded either “not at all” or “slightly”to the question of “How serious do you feel your present legal problems are?” There were no statistically significant differences in responses to this question for female and male participants. Last, 18 (51.4%) of the 35 participants were on probation or parole at the time of the study. Probation or parole status differed significantly for female and male participants: 8 (72.7%) of the 11 female participants were on probation or parole in comparison to 9 (37.5%) of the 24 male participants (χ2 (1) = 3.75; p = .05).

Family and Social Status

Of the 35 participants, 21 were never married, 12 were divorced, and 2 were widowed. Twenty- seven (77.1%) participants expressed satisfaction with their current relationship status. Sixteen participants (46%) reported living in a controlled environment, nine (25.7%) lived alone, four with significant others, three with family, one with children alone, one with parents, and one reported no stable living arrangement. Twenty (57.1%) participants reported being satisfied with their current living arrangements and 15 (42.9%) participants were not. Participants were asked the number of days in the past 30 days with serious family conflict. They estimated a mean of 1.37 (5.59) days in the past month with serious family conflict, with four participants (11.4%) reporting being extremely concerned about family conflict. For serious social conflict, participants estimated on average 1.68 (4.51) days in the past month, with six (17.6%) participants reporting being extremely disturbed about social conflict. There were no statistically significant differences between men and women on these measures of satisfaction and measures of number of days in the past month with incidents of either family or social conflict.

Twenty-eight (82.4%) participants expressed satisfaction with their free time, and 17 (50%) spent most of their free time with family, 9 stayed (26. 5%) alone, and 8 spent time (23.5%) with friends. A chi-square test of linear-by-linear association was performed to test satisfaction with free time in relation to whether such time was spent with family, friends, or alone. The data revealed a significant linear association between satisfaction and spending free time with family in contrast to dissatisfaction with spending free time alone (χ2 (1) = 7.94; p = .005).

Psychiatric Status

In the past 30 days, 50 percent of the participants reported experiencing symptoms of depression, and 62.9 percent responded affirmatively to the question of current serious anxiety, tension, or unreasonable worry. Three (8.6%) participants reported experiencing hallucinations in the past 30 days. For the question that asked participants if they have had trouble understanding or thinking in the past 30 days, 11 (31.4%) answered “yes.” Participants estimated on average 8.42 (9.86) days over the past 30 days of experiencing any of these problems (depression, anxiety, hallucinations, and thinking). Participants were also asked about problems controlling violent behavior over the past 30 days and over their lifetimes. Twenty-eight (80%) participants reported having no problem controlling violent behavior over the past 30 days. By comparison, 27 (77.1%) participants reported having problems controlling violent behavior over their lifetimes.

With regard to gender differences, more men (60.9%) reported depression in the past 30 days than did women (27.4%), and this difference approached statistical significance (χ2 (1) = 3.36; p = .067). Male and female participants also differed in reported hallucinations, not in the past 30 days, but over their lifetimes. Sixteen men (66.7%) reported a lifetime history of hallucinations in contrast to none (0%) of the female group, a statistically significant difference (χ2 (1) = 4.75, p = .029). The chi-square test also revealed significant group differences in percentages of the men and women who reported problems controlling violent behavior in their lifetimes: 21 (87.5%) men versus 6 (54.5%) women reported lifetime problems controlling their behavior (χ2 (1) = 4.65; p = 031). However, the data revealed no significant correlation between self-reported hallucinations and lifetime use of hallucinogens. Results also revealed treatment differences with respect to gender, with 77.8 percent of female participants prescribed medications for emotional problems in the past 30 days in comparison to 28.6 percent of the male group (χ2 (1) = 6.12; p = .013).

We used four ASI items that asked participants to answer “yes” or “no” as to whether they had in the last 30 days experienced problems with depression, anxiety, thinking, and hallucinations. Their responses, when subjected to a mixed-model ANOVA with one between-subjects factor of gender (male/female) and one within-subjects factor of psychological profile with four levels (depression, anxiety, thinking, and hallucinations), revealed a statistically significant effect for the interaction of gender × psychological profile (F (3,96) = 2.82; p = .04; partial η2 = 0.081). This statistically significant interaction indicated that male and female participants presented with different psychological profiles. Men presented with higher rates of problems with depression and anxiety, in comparison with higher rates of problems with thinking for women. Male and female participants also showed different patterns of lifetime histories for these psychological symptoms, as revealed by a statistically significant interaction of gender by lifetime history of psychological symptoms (F (3, 99) = 3.46; p = .019; partial η2 = .095). Both groups had similar rates of a history of problems with depression and anxiety, but the women reported higher rates of lifetime problems with thinking in comparison to the men who reported higher lifetime rates of problems with hallucinations.

Neuropsychology

The overall group performed in the lower end of the mean (SD) range on the oral reading subtest of the WRAT4 (92.17 (12.45)), which is often used as a proxy measure of premorbid intelligence. On the IGT, the overall group also showed evidence of learning as reflected by improved performance over time on task (F (3,102) = 25.04; p < .001; partial η2 = .424). However, as shown in Fig. 1, IGT performance improved over the first three blocks, but declined over the last two, pointing to a failure to sustain gains. IGT performance did not differ for 16 participants on parole or probation versus the 18 who were not on parole or probation, but did differ for participants (n = 17) who reported experiencing serious depression or sadness over the past 30 days versus those who did not (n = 17). That is, a mixed-model analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) with between-subjects factors of parole/probation status (yes/no) and depressive symptoms (yes/no), with one within-subject factor of IGT performance (blocks 1–5), ANCOVA for gender revealed a significant depressive symptoms × IGT performance interaction (F (4,116) = 2.91; p = .025; partial η2 = .091). As shown in Fig. 2, participants with reported depressive symptoms, despite performing worse than their counterparts for blocks two and three, had higher earnings for the remaining two IGT blocks.

Figure 1.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 1.

Iowa Gambling Task performance of ex-offenders.

Figure 2.
  • Download figure
  • Open in new tab
  • Download powerpoint
Figure 2.

Iowa Gambling Task performance as a function of reported depression, sadness, or loss of interest in the past 30 days.

Likewise, a mixed-model ANCOVA with one between-subjects factor of gender (male/female) one within-subject factor of IGT performance with five levels (blocks 1–5), and a covariate of parole/probation status on which the two groups differed, revealed a marginally significant effect for gender (F (1, 32) = 3.93; p = .05; η2 =.109). As shown in Fig. 1, men and women showed similar levels of performance on IGT trial blocks two and three, but the women's performance declined over the last two trial blocks, leading to a lower score. We next examined social influences on IGT performance. Twenty-five participants reported spending most of their free time with family or friends, and the remaining nine participants reported spending their free time alone. We then subjected IGT performance for blocks 1 and 5 to a mixed-model ANOVA with one between-subjects factor of free time (alone, others) and one within-subject factor of IGT (blocks 1 and 5). This ANOVA revealed a significant interaction of free time × IGT block (F (1, 32) = 6.86; p = .013; partial η2 = .176). This interaction pointed to better performance for block 1 for those who spent most of their free time with others (mean (SD)) (−1.52 (7.17)) versus alone (−9.11 (9.12)), but the opposite pattern for block 5 with the free time spent alone group (2.67 (13.96)) outperforming free time spent with others (−0.400 (7.83)).

Last, we calculated the median IGT total net (median −$705.00) and divided the sample into high (n = 18) and low (n = 17) IGT performers. High and low IGT performers reported different patterns of lifetime arrests for burglary or larceny versus driving while under the influence, as reflected by the significant interaction of group (high and low IGT) by lifetime arrests (burglary and larceny and driving under the influence) (F (1, 33) = 4.36; p = .045; partial η2 = .117). Low IGT performers reported prior criminal records of a higher number of arrests for burglary or larceny and a lower number for driving under the influence in comparison to the high IGT performers who showed the opposite pattern. The high and low IGT performers also showed different patterns of lifetime drug use for heroin, cocaine, amphetamines, sedative hypnotics, and hallucinogens. That is, ANCOVA with gender as a covariate revealed significant group × lifetime drug use (F (4, 128) = 2.93; p = .024; partial η2 = .084). The low IGT group had greater lifetime rates of heroin, cocaine, and sedative hypnotics, in comparison to greater lifetime rates of amphetamines and hallucinogens for the high IGT group.

Discussion

In the current study, we used a social cognitive framework that combined the ASI-CF and the IGT to examine the dynamics of addiction and criminality in male and female ex-offenders who were recently released from jail or prison and who were recruited from a community-based offender re-entry service program. The results confirmed the long and complicated histories of substance use disorders and criminality in this sample of treatment-resistant, repeat offenders. Indeed, participants had lengthy histories of alcohol, cannabis, cocaine, and heroin addiction, and had spent, on average, a mean lifetime total of 11.4 years incarcerated, primarily for repeated arrests for misdemeanor charges. Notwithstanding their past difficulties, participants, in general, reported a degree of satisfaction with their current circumstances, such as their living arrangements, their use of free time, and their interpersonal relationships. In addition, participants as a group did not express concern about their legal problems, although 17 reported that they were on probation or parole status at the time of the current study. However, our analyses were limited, as we did not distinguish between probation and parole status, nor did we specify the charges underlying probation or parole.

On the IGT, participants showed evidence of reward-learning across the initial three blocks of 20 trials, peaking in the third bock in trials 40 to 60. However, their performance declined over the last 40 trials, suggesting a failure to sustain gains: that is, to learn from feedback in deciding among advantageous and disadvantageous decks of cards. Women also performed more poorly on IGT than did men, a finding that has been reported in other studies using nonforensic samples without histories as offenders. In the current study, however, the small sample size of women substantially limits this finding.32 Nonetheless, the overall IGT findings are consistent with prior studies, pointing to evidence of a substance abuse–related disturbance in motivated decision-making. Neuroscientific evidence has suggested that intact IGT performance depends heavily on the orbital frontal lobe connections (OFCs) to the striatum that are vital to instrumental reward-learning,20,33,34 and those can be disrupted by substance abuse.35 Thus, in the current study, that these decision-making deficits emerged on the last two blocks of IGT trials may be viewed as support for an OFC-mediated failure to sustain a proper balance of reward and punishment sensitivity that is essential for learning and sound judgment.

The findings also suggest that depression and social factors influence IGT performance, but each showed a very distinct, if not paradoxical pattern, as reflected by statistically significant interactions. For example, participants with lower levels of self-reported depression had higher earnings for the second and third blocks of IGT trials, but lower earnings for the last two blocks of trials. By contrast, those reporting higher levels of depression showed the opposite pattern, with lower earnings on the second and third blocks but finishing with higher earnings across the final two blocks of trials. Hence, the participants who endorsed feelings of depression showed evidence of learning with better performance over the last two blocks of IGT trials. This paradoxical finding may be best understood as sad feelings serving as motivation to improve, which in turn may be expected to be associated with better learning on the IGT. In a similar vein, poorer IGT performance may have been associated with reduced motivation and limited self-awareness in those who are not emotionally troubled or saddened by their current circumstances of lengthy histories of substance abuse, criminal involvement, repeated arrests, and incarceration. In this regard, then, the simple dichotomous ASI-CF item of depression is perhaps best viewed as tapping normal sadness rather than clinical depression.

The results point to a similar pattern of IGT performance in relation to social factors, as measured by the ASI-CF item of spending free time with others or alone. Participants who reported spending their free time with others performed better on the first block of IGT trials but worse on the last block of trials, whereas those who spent their free time alone showed the opposite pattern of performance, beginning worse but ending with significantly higher earnings on the last block. These data indicated improved IGT performance for participants who preferred to spend their free time alone in comparison to those who chose to spend their free time with others. As emphasized in the recent NIMH initiative, known as the Research Domain Criteria, various clinical disorders may have distinct social developmental pathways. In this regard, substance abuse and criminality often first emerge among peers in adolescence, and these problems can be fueled by social pressures in ways that are very different from other developmental disorders that often first occur in adolescence, such as psychotic at-risk states and prodromal schizophrenia. These latter conditions are often characterized by social anhedonia, which is a loss of appetite for engaging with others, leading to isolation and emotional withdrawal. Thus, the results suggested that for this sample of ex-offenders, spending free time alone, which presumably reflected less social connection and engagement than did spending free time with others, may actually have had a beneficial effect in motivated decision-making as measured by the IGT.

With respect to psychological profiles extracted from the ASI-CF, problems with both anxiety and depression over the past 30 days prevailed in most of the sample, with highest reports for anxiety. By contrast, relatively fewer participants reported problems with cognition or hallucinations in the past 30 days. Likewise, only a few participants (20%) reported problems with violent behavior over the past 30 days. Lifetime histories of these psychological symptoms and behavioral problems, however, showed gender differences. For example, whereas both groups reported histories of depression and anxiety, women endorsed higher rates of lifetime problems with thinking in comparison to men who reported higher lifetime rates of problems with hallucinations. For men, there was no relationship between their histories of hallucinations and past or current use of hallucinogens. This result suggested that their reported hallucinations may have been indicative of psychosis occurring within the context of a co-occurring substance abuse and mental illness and would be consistent with the strikingly elevated rates of co-occurring substance abuse disorder and mental illness in prison populations.2

For women, they endorsed the ASI-CF problems-with-thinking item that asks about cognitive difficulties with concentration, understanding, and remembering. For women, these difficulties may have represented cognitive sequelae of their long histories of drug abuse that have included opiates, stimulants, and hallucinogens. As already noted, women also performed more poorly than men on the IGT. In fact, their IGT performance was very similar to that of male recidivists from our prior study of ex-offenders who had been recently released from jail or prison and were receiving services from the same ex-offender re-entry agency from which the current participants were drawn.22 Finally, IGT performance also correlated with different patterns of lifetime histories of both drug abuse and criminality. For example, the data pointed to associations between poorer performance on the IGT with greater lifetime rates of heroin, cocaine, and sedative hypnotics. Criminality also differed as a function of IGT performance. Low IGT performance was linked to higher arrests for burglary, in comparison to higher arrests for driving under the influence for high IGT performance.

In summary, the current study, while lacking follow-up data, again provided strong evidence for the ecological validity of the IGT, coupled with the ASI-CF in explicating the role of decision-making in participants with long and complicated histories of substance use disorders and criminality. Indeed, the current data showed for the first time, to the best of our knowledge, the moderating effects of both social behavior, as measured by the ASI, and gender on IGT performance in this sample of substance abuse, ex-criminal offenders. As such, the current investigation offered support for integrating neuropsychological and semistructured interview measures to examine through the lens of social cognition the relationship of addiction and criminality. However, future studies are needed with larger samples of male and female participants, both to examine the generalizability of the current findings and to plumb more deeply into how gender may influence the social–cognitive dynamics of addiction and criminality.

Footnotes

  • Disclosures of financial or other potential conflicts of interest: None.

  • © 2018 American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law

References

  1. 1.↵
    1. Regier DA,
    2. Narrow WE,
    3. Rae DS
    : The epidemiology of anxiety disorders: the epidemiologic catchment area (ECA) experience. J Psychiatr Res 24:3–14, 1990
    OpenUrlPubMed
  2. 2.↵
    National Institutes of Health: Fact Sheet. Addiction and the Criminal Justice System, October 2010. Available at: https://report.nih.gov/nihfactsheets/Pdfs/AddictionandtheCriminalJusticeSystem(NIDA).pdf. Accessed September 5, 2016
  3. 3.↵
    1. Gordon MS,
    2. Kinlock TW,
    3. Schwartz RP,
    4. et al
    : A randomized clinical trial of methadone maintenance for prisoners: findings at 6 months post-release. Addiction 103:1333–42, 2008
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  4. 4.↵
    1. Gordon MS,
    2. Kinlock TW,
    3. Couvillion KA,
    4. et al
    : A randomized clinical trial of methadone maintenance for prisoners: prediction of treatment entry and completion in prison. J Offender Rehabil 51:222–38, 2012
    OpenUrl
  5. 5.↵
    1. Kinlock TW,
    2. Gordon MS,
    3. Schwartz RP,
    4. et al
    : A randomized clinical trial of methadone maintenance for prisoners: results at 12 months post-release. J Subst Abuse Treat 37:277–85, 2009
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  6. 6.↵
    1. Kinlock TW,
    2. Gordon MS,
    3. Schwartz RP,
    4. et al
    : Individual patient and program factors related to prison and community treatment completion in prison-initiated methadone maintenance treatment. J Offender Rehabil 52:509–28, 2013
    OpenUrl
  7. 7.↵
    1. Cuthbert BN,
    2. Insel TR,
    3. Wing L,
    4. et al
    : Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Med 11:126, 2013
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  8. 8.↵
    1. Insel TR
    : A neurobiological basis of social attachment. Am J Psychiatry 154:726–35, 1997
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  9. 9.↵
    1. Krueger F,
    2. Parasuraman R,
    3. Iyengar V,
    4. et al
    : Oxytocin receptor genetic variation promotes human trust behavior. Front Hum Neurosci 6:1–4, 2012
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  10. 10.↵
    1. Hyman S
    : The diagnosis of mental disorders: the problem with reification. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 6:155–79, 2010
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  11. 11.↵
    1. Buckholtz JW,
    2. Meyer-Lindenberg A
    : Psychopathology and the human connectome: toward a transdiagnostic model of risk for mental illness. Neuron 74:990–1004, 2012
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  12. 12.↵
    1. Frith CD
    : Social cognition. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 363:2033–39, 2008
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  13. 13.↵
    1. Winkielman P,
    2. Schooler JW
    : Splitting consciousness: unconscious, conscious, and metaconscious processes in social cognition. Eur Rev Soc Psychol 22:1–35, 2011
    OpenUrl
  14. 14.↵
    1. McLellan AT,
    2. Luborsky L,
    3. Woody GE,
    4. et al
    : An improved diagnostic evaluation instrument for substance abuse patients: the addiction severity index. J Nerv Ment Dis 168:26–33, 1980
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  15. 15.↵
    1. Bechara A,
    2. Damasio AR,
    3. Damasio H,
    4. et al
    : Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition 50:7–15, 1994
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  16. 16.↵
    1. Forsyth JK,
    2. Bachman P,
    3. Mathalon DH,
    4. et al
    : Augmenting NMDA receptor signaling boosts experience-dependent neuroplasticity in the adult human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 112:15331–36, 2015
    OpenUrlAbstract/FREE Full Text
  17. 17.↵
    1. Bechara A,
    2. Damasio H
    : Decision-making and addiction (part II): myopia for the future or hypersensitivity to reward? Neuropsychologia 40:1690–705, 2002
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  18. 18.↵
    1. Grant S,
    2. Contoreggi C,
    3. London ED
    : Drug abusers show impaired performance in a laboratory test of decision making. Neuropsychologia 38:1180–87, 2000
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  19. 19.↵
    1. Toplak ME,
    2. Sorge GB,
    3. Benoit A,
    4. et al
    : Decision-making and cognitive abilities: a review of associations between Iowa gambling task performance, executive functions, and intelligence. Clin Psychol Rev 30:562–81, 2010
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  20. 20.↵
    1. Nestor PG,
    2. Niznikiewicz M,
    3. McCarley RW
    : Distinct contribution of working memory and social comprehension failures in neuropsychological impairment in schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis 198:206–12, 2010
    OpenUrlPubMed
  21. 21.↵
    1. Sevy S,
    2. Burdick KE,
    3. Visweswaraiah H,
    4. et al
    : Iowa Gambling Task in schizophrenia: a review and new data in patients with schizophrenia and co-occurring cannabis use disorders. Schizophr Res 92:74–84, 2007
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  22. 22.↵
    1. Beszterczey S,
    2. Nestor PG,
    3. Shirai A,
    4. et al
    : Neuropsychology of decision making and psychopathy in high-risk ex-offenders. Neuropsychology 27:491–97, 2013
    OpenUrl
  23. 23.↵
    1. Mäkelä K
    : Studies of the reliability and validity of the addiction severity index. Addiction 99:398–410, 2004
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  24. 24.↵
    1. Alterman AI,
    2. Bovasso GB,
    3. Cacciola JS,
    4. et al
    : A comparison of the predictive validity of four sets of baseline ASI summary indices. Psychol Addict Behav 15:159–62, 2001
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  25. 25.↵
    1. Daeppen JB,
    2. Burnand B,
    3. Schnyder C,
    4. et al
    : Validation of the Addiction Severity Index in French-speaking alcoholic patients. J Stud Alcohol 57:585–90, 1996
    OpenUrlPubMed
  26. 26.↵
    1. DeJong CA,
    2. Willems JC,
    3. Schippers GM,
    4. et al
    : The addiction severity index: reliability and validity in a Dutch alcoholic population. Int J Addict 30:605–16, 1995
    OpenUrlPubMed
  27. 27.↵
    1. McDermott P,
    2. Alterman A
    : Construct refinement and confirmation for the Addiction Severity Index. Psychol Assess 8:182–89, 1996
    OpenUrlCrossRef
  28. 28.↵
    1. Zanis DA,
    2. McLellan AT,
    3. Alterman AI,
    4. et al
    : Efficacy of enhanced outreach counseling to reenroll high-risk drug users 1 year after discharge from treatment. Am J Psychiatry 153:1095–96, 1996
    OpenUrlPubMed
  29. 29.↵
    1. Cacciola JS,
    2. Alterman AI,
    3. McLellan AT,
    4. et al
    : Initial evidence for the reliability and validity of a “lite” version of the addiction severity index. Drug Alcohol Depend 87:297–302, 2007
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  30. 30.↵
    1. Rodríguez-Sánchez JM,
    2. Crespo-Facorro B,
    3. Iglesias RP,
    4. et al
    : Prefrontal cognitive functions in stabilized first-episode patients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders: a dissociation between dorsolateral and orbitofrontal functioning. Schizophr Res 77:279–88, 2005
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  31. 31.↵
    1. Wilkinson GS,
    2. Robertson GJ
    : Wide Range Achievement Test (ed 4). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, 2006
  32. 32.↵
    1. Overman WH,
    2. Pierce A
    : Iowa gambling task with non-clinical participants: Effects of using real + virtual cards and additional trials. Front Psychol 4:935–50, 2013
    OpenUrl
  33. 33.↵
    1. Kringelbach ML,
    2. Radcliffe J
    : The human orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to hedonic experience. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:691–702, 2005
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  34. 34.↵
    1. Nakamura M,
    2. Nestor PG,
    3. Levitt JJ,
    4. et al
    : Orbitofrontal volume deficit in schizophrenia and thought disorder. Brain 131:180–95, 2008
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
  35. 35.↵
    1. Volkow ND,
    2. Li T-K
    . Drug addiction: The neurobiology of behaviour gone awry. Nat Rev Neurosci 5:963–70, 2004
    OpenUrlCrossRefPubMed
PreviousNext
Back to top

In this issue

Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online: 46 (2)
Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online
Vol. 46, Issue 2
1 Jun 2018
  • Table of Contents
  • Index by author
Print
Download PDF
Article Alerts
Sign In to Email Alerts with your Email Address
Email Article

Thank you for your interest in recommending The Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law site.

NOTE: We only request your email address so that the person you are recommending the page to knows that you wanted them to see it, and that it is not junk mail. We do not capture any email address.

Enter multiple addresses on separate lines or separate them with commas.
Clinical, Social, and Neuropsychological Dimensions of the Intersection of Addiction and Criminality
(Your Name) has forwarded a page to you from Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law
(Your Name) thought you would like to see this page from the Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law web site.
CAPTCHA
This question is for testing whether or not you are a human visitor and to prevent automated spam submissions.
Citation Tools
Clinical, Social, and Neuropsychological Dimensions of the Intersection of Addiction and Criminality
Paul G. Nestor, Ashley Woodhull, Dominick Newell, Keira O'Donovan, Mayte Forte, Sean Harding, Marc Pomplun
Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online Jun 2018, 46 (2) 179-186; DOI: 10.29158/JAAPL.003745-18

Citation Manager Formats

  • BibTeX
  • Bookends
  • EasyBib
  • EndNote (tagged)
  • EndNote 8 (xml)
  • Medlars
  • Mendeley
  • Papers
  • RefWorks Tagged
  • Ref Manager
  • RIS
  • Zotero

Share
Clinical, Social, and Neuropsychological Dimensions of the Intersection of Addiction and Criminality
Paul G. Nestor, Ashley Woodhull, Dominick Newell, Keira O'Donovan, Mayte Forte, Sean Harding, Marc Pomplun
Journal of the American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law Online Jun 2018, 46 (2) 179-186; DOI: 10.29158/JAAPL.003745-18
del.icio.us logo Twitter logo Facebook logo Mendeley logo
  • Tweet Widget
  • Facebook Like
  • Google Plus One

Jump to section

  • Article
    • Abstract
    • Method
    • Results
    • Discussion
    • Footnotes
    • References
  • Figures & Data
  • Info & Metrics
  • PDF

Related Articles

Cited By...

More in this TOC Section

  • Attitudes of Forensic Fellowship Psychiatry Directors towards an Applicant Match
  • Suicide Prevention Effects of Extreme Risk Protection Order Laws in Four States
  • Mental Health and Social Correlates of Reincarceration of Youths as Adults
Show more Regular Articles

Similar Articles

Site Navigation

  • Home
  • Current Issue
  • Ahead of Print
  • Archive
  • Information for Authors
  • About the Journal
  • Editorial Board
  • Feedback
  • Alerts

Other Resources

  • Academy Website
  • AAPL Meetings
  • AAPL Annual Review Course

Reviewers

  • Peer Reviewers

Other Publications

  • AAPL Practice Guidelines
  • AAPL Newsletter
  • AAPL Ethics Guidelines
  • AAPL Amicus Briefs
  • Landmark Cases

Customer Service

  • Cookie Policy
  • Reprints and Permissions
  • Order Physical Copy

Copyright © 2025 by The American Academy of Psychiatry and the Law